The Production of Consciousness out of States of Consciousness
© 1999
Todd Murphy
E-mail:
Brainsci@jps.net
Return Home
Neuromagnetic Signals as the Basis for States of Consciousness
In what could turn out to be one of the most important discoveries in cognitive studies of our decade, it has been
found that there are five million magnetite crystals per gram in the human brain (1). Interestingly, The meninges,
(the membrane that envelops the brain), has twenty times that number. These "biomagnetite' crystals demonstrate
two interesting features. The first is that their shapes do not occur in nature, suggesting that they were formed
in the tissue, rather than being absorbed from outside. The other is that these crystals appear to be oriented
so as to maximize their magnetic moment, which tends to give groups of these crystals the capacity to act as a
system. The brain has also been found to emit very low intensity magnetic fields, a phenomenon that forms the basis
of a whole diagnostic field, Magnetoencephalography(2).
Unfortunately for the present discussion, there is no way to "read' any signals that might be carried by the
brains magnetic emissions at present. We expect that subtle enough means of detecting such signals will eventually
appear, as there is compelling evidence that they do exist, and constitute a means whereby communication happens
between various parts of the brain. This system, we speculate, is what makes the selection of which neural areas
to recruit, so that States (of consciousness) can elicit the appropriate phenomenological, behavioral, and affective
responses.
While there have been many studies that have examined the effects of magnetic fields on human consciousness, none
have yielded findings more germane to understanding the role of neuromagnetic signaling than the work of the Laurentian
University Behavioral Neurosciences group. They have pursued a course of experiments that rely on stimulating the
brain, especially the temporal lobes, with complex low intensity magnetic signals (3). It turns out that different
signals produce different phenomena.
One example of such phenomenon is vestibular sensation, in which one's normal sense of balance is replaced by illusions
of motion similar to the feelings of levitation reported in spiritual literature as well as the sensation of vertigo.
Transient "visions', whose content includes motifs that also appear in near-death experiences and alien abduction
scenarios have also appeared (4). Positive affectual parasthesias (electric-like buzzes in the body) have occurred.
Another experience that has been elicited neuromagnetically is bursts of emotion, most commonly fear and joy. Although
the content of these experiences can be quite striking, the way they present themselves is much more ordinary.
It approximates the "twilight state' between waking and sleep called hypnogogia. This can produce brief, fleeting
visions, feelings that the bed is moving, rocking, floating or sinking. Electric-buzz like somatic sensations and
hearing an inner voice call one's name can also occur in hypnogogia. The range of experiences it can produce is
quite broad. If all signals produced the same phenomena, then it would be difficult to conclude that these magnetic
signals approximate the postulated endogenous neuromagnetic signals that create alterations in State. In fact,
the former produce a wide variety of phenomena. One such signal makes some women apprehensive, another doesn't
(5). One signal creates such strong vestibular sensations that one can't stand up. Another doesn't.
The temporal lobes are the parts of the brain that mediate states of consciousness. EEG readouts from the temporal
lobes are markedly different when a person is asleep, having a hallucinogenic seizure, or on LSD. Siezural disorders
confined to the temporal lobes (complex partial seizures) have been characterized as impairments of consciousness
(6). There was also a study done in which monkeys were given LSD after having various parts of their brains removed
(7). The monkeys continued to "trip' no matter what part or parts of their brains were missing until both
temporal lobes were taken out. In these cases, the substance did not seem to affect the monkeys at all. The conclusion
seems unavoidable. In addition to all their other functions (aspects of memory, language, music, etc.), the temporal
lobes mediate states of consciousness.
If exposing the temporal lobes to magnetic signals can induce alterations in States, then it seems reasonable to
suppose that States find part of their neural basis in our postulated neuromagnetic signals, arising out of the
temporal lobes.
Hallucinations are known to be the phenomenological correlates of altered States. Alterations in state of consciousness
leads, following input, and phenomena, whether hallucinatory or not, follows in response. We can offer two reasons
for drawing this conclusion.
The first is one of the results obtained by a study of hallucinations caused by electrical stimulation deep in
the brain (8). In this study, the content of the hallucinations was found to be related to the circumstances in
which they occurred, so that the same stimulations could produce different hallucinations. The conclusion was that
the stimulation induced altered states, and the states facilitated the hallucinations.
The second has to do with the relative speeds of the operant neural processes.
Neurochemical response times are limited by the time required for their transmission across the synaptic gap, .5
to 2msec (9).
By comparison, the propagation of action potentials is much faster. For example, an action potential can travel
a full centimeter (a couple of orders of magnitude larger than a synaptic gap) in about 1.3 msec. The brain's electrical
responses, therefore, happen orders of magnitude more quickly than do it's chemical ones (10).
Magnetic signals are propagated with much greater speeds than those of action potentials moving through neurons.
Contemporary physics requires that magnetic signals be propagated at a significant fraction of the velocity of
light, so that the entire brain could be exposed to a neuromagnetic signal in vanishingly small amounts of time.
It seems possible that neuromagnetic signals arise from structures which mediate our various sensory and cognitive
modalities. These signals then recruit those functions (primarily in the limbic system) that adjust the changes
in state. These temporal lobe signals, we speculate, then initiate signals to structures that mediate modalities
that are enhanced or suppressed as the state changes.
Consciousness as a Feedback Interface of Sensory
and Cognitive Modalities
The problem of defining the phrase "state of consciousness' has plagued the field of cognitive studies for
some time. Without going into the history of studies in the area, we would like to outline an hypothesis concerning
states of consciousness in which the management of states gives rise to the phenomenon of consciousness.
There are theories that suggest that cognitive modalities (such as memory, affect, ideation and attention) may
be seen as analogs to sensory modalities.
We hypothesize that the entire set of modalities, cognitive and sensory, may be heuristically compared to a sound
mixing board. In this metaphor, all the various modalities are represented as vertical rheostats with enhanced
functioning increasing towards the top, and suppressed function increasing towards the bottom. Further, the act
of becoming conscious of phenomena in any given modality involves the adjustment of that modality's "rheostat'.
Sensory input from any modality can alter one's state. The sight of a sexy person, the smell of fire, the unexpected
sensation of movement against one's skin (there's a bug on me!), a sudden bitter taste experienced while eating
ice cream, or the sound of one's child screaming in pain; all of these phenomena can induce alterations in State.
Although the phrase "altered states' has come to be associated with dramatic, otherworldly experiences, alterations
in state, as we will be using the phrase, refers primarily to those alterations that take us from one normal state
to another.
Alterations in state can create changes within the various sensory and cognitive modalities. An increase in arousal
following the sight of a predator will typically suppress the sense of smell (very few are able to stop and "smell
the roses' while a jaguar is chasing them), suppress introspection (nobody wants to know "who am I really?'
while an anaconda is wrapping itself around them), suppress sexual arousal, and alter vision so that the center
of the visual field is better attended then one's peripheral vision allowing one to see the predator's movement
better. The sight of a predator will also introduce a host of other changes, all of which reflect the State.
In the Hindu epic, the Mahabharata, there is a dialog between the legendary warrior, Arjuna, and his archery teacher.
Arjuna was told by his teacher to train his bow on a straw bird used as a target. Arjuna was asked to describe
the bird. He answered "I can't'. "Why not?', asked his teacher. "I can only see its eye', he answered.
"Release your arrow', commanded the teacher. Arjuna did, and hit the target in the eye. "I'll make you
the finest archer in the world', said his teacher.
In this story, attention to peripheral vision had ceased so completely that only the very center of his visual
field received any. Our model of states would be constrained to interpret Arjuna's (mythical) feat as a behavior
specific to a state. The unique combination of sensory enhancement, heightened attention, and sufficient suppression
of emotion, ideation, and introspection that support such an act suggests specific settings for our metaphorical
rheostats.
Changes in state make changes in sensory and cognitive modalities, and they in turn, trigger changes in state.
We can reasonably conclude that there is a feedback mechanism whereby each modality is connected to the others.
States also create tendencies to behave in specific ways in specific circumstances, maximizing the adaptivity of
behavior in those circumstances; behavior that tends to meet our needs and respond to threats to our ability to
meet those needs.
Each circumstance adjusts each modalities' setting, tending to maximize that modality's contribution to adaptive
behavior in that circumstance. The mechanism may function by using both learned and inherited default settings
for each circumstance and then repeating those settings in similar circumstances later on. Sadly, this often makes
states maladaptive. An habituated alteration in State, in response to threats from an abusive parent, for example,
can make for self-defeating responses to stress in other circumstances, where theses same responses are no longer
advantageous (10).
Because different States are going to be dominated by specific combinations of modalities, it stands to reason
that a possible strategy for aligning the rheostats (making alterations in state) is to move them in tandem, so
that after a person associates the sound of a scream to the concept of a threat, that sound, with it's unique auditory
signature, will cause all the affected modalities (most likely most of them in most cases) to take the positions
they had at the time the association was made.
When we say changing states, we are referring to much more than the dramatic states created by LSD, isolation tanks,
REM sleep, etc. We are also including normal states of consciousness, which we can imagine as kindled "default
settings' of our various modalities. When any one of these settings returns to one of its default settings, it
will, we conjecture, tend to entrain all the other modalities to the settings they habitually take in that state.
To accomplish this, we must suggest that each modality is connected to every other one. A sight, a smell, a sound,
or a tactile feeling can all inspire fear. Fear can motivate ideation. Ideation can inspire arousal. Changes in
affect can initiate alterations in introspection. Introspection alters affect. State specific settings of individual
modalities could initiate settings for other modalities.
Our main hypothesis here is that all these intermodal connections, operating as a single system, has a single phenomenological
correlate. The phenomena of subjective awareness.
We proposed in our first section that the alteration of consciousness involves having a modality receive input
that triggers a change in State. The structure associated with that modality then broadcasts a neuromagnetic signal
to the temporal lobes, which then produces signals that then recruits various structures throughout the brain.
Specifically, those structures whose associated modalities' values must be changed in order to accomplish the appropriate
alteration in state. In the second section, we found the possibility that states are settings for the variable
aspects of cognitive and sensory modalities. We also offered the suggestion that consciousness is the phenomenological
correlate of the feedback between the management of states on the one hand, and the various cognitive and sensory
modalities, on the other. If all of these conclusions were to stand up to testing, we could conclude that the content
of the brain's hypothesized endogenous magnetic signals might consist of a set of values for adjusting each sensory
and cognitive rheostat. We might also conclude that neuromagnetic signaling is the context in which consciousness
occurs.
The specific mechanism whereby subjectivity is generated is out of the reach of this work. Nevertheless, we can
say that the fact that multiple modalities are experienced simultaneously, together with our model's implication
that they are "reset,' all at once, with each alteration in state suggests that our postulated neuromagnetic
signals may come in pairs, with the two signals running slightly out of phase with one another. In this way, neuromagnatic
signals, like the two laser beams used to produce a hologram, might be able to store information in a similar way,
as has already been explored by Karl Pibhram. The speeds at which neuromagnetic signals are propagated, together
with their capacity to recruit/alter multiple modalities suggests that the underlying mechanism has been selected
to make instant choices on which specific portions to recruit in order to facilitate the behaviors acted out of
the State, and to do so quickly.
In this way, the onset time for the initiation of States is kept to a minimum, and with it, the times needed to
make the initial, cognitive response to stimuli. When it comes to response to threats, or sighting prey, the evolutionary
advantages are obvious.
Return Home
Email your comments.
REFERENCES
(1) Kirshivink, Joseph L, Kobayashi-Kirshivink, Atsuko & Woodford, Barbera J., "Magnetite Biomineralization
in the Human Brain" Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 1992, 89 7683-7687
(2) cf. Stefan, H. Abraham-Fuchs, K., Shnieder, S., Gebhardt, M. Neubauer, U. Hummel, C., , Huk, W.J., & Thierauf
P., "Magnetic Source Localization and Morphological Changes in Temporal Lobe Epilepsy: Comparison of MEG/EEG,
EcoG and Volumatric MRI in Pesurgical Evaluation of Operated Patients" Neurologia Scandinavica. Supplementum.
1994, 152 83-8
(3) Ruttan, Leslie A., Persinger, Michael A, & Koren, Stanley, "Enhancement of Temporal Lobe-Related Experiences
During Brief Exposures to Milligaus Intensity Extremely Low Intensity Magnetc Fields" Journal of Bioelectricity
9 (1) 33-54, 1990
(4) Persinger, Michael A., Ph.D. "Near-Death Experiences: Determining the Neuroanatomical Pathways by Experiential
Patterns and Simulation In Experimental Settings", Appeared in: Healing: Beyond Suffering and death. Publications
MNH, 1993
(5) Richards, P.M., Persinger, M.A. & Koren, Stanley, "Experimental Stimulation by Burst-Firing Weak Magnetic
Fields over the Right Temporal Lobe May Facilitate Apprehension in Women." Perceptual and Motor Skill, 1992,
75, 667-670
(6) Feldman, Robert G. "Complex Partial Siezures (Psychomotor or Temporal Lobe Siezures)" Appeared in
Epilepsy- Diagnosis and Management (Brown, Thomas R., M.D.. & Feldman, Robert G. M.D., Little, Brown &
Co., 1983
(7) Baldwin, M., "Neurologic Syndromes and Hallucinations." Appeared in: Origin and Mechanisms of Hallucinations.
Keup, Wolfram, (Ed.) Plenum Press, 1970
(8) Horowitz, M.J. & Adams, J.E. "Hallucinations on Brain Stimulation: Evidence for the Revision of the
Penfield Hypothesis." Appeared in: Keup, (Ed.) Origin and Mechanism of Hallucinations Plenum Press, 1970
(9) Stevens, Charles F. "The Neuron" Appeared in : The Brain W.H. Freeman and Co., 1979
(10) Kalat, James W. "Biological Psychology (2nd Ed.)" Pg. 46, Wadsworth Publishing Co. 1981
(11) Perry, Bruce D., M.D., Pollard, Ronnie A., Blakley, Toi L., Baker, William L., Vigilante, Domenico "Childhood
Trauma, The Neurobiology of Adaptation, and the "use dependent' Development of the Brain: How "States'
become "traits.' " Infant Mental Health Journal Vol. 16, No. 4, Winter 1995
Other pages on this site:
Shakti - Magnetic Brain Stimulation
Origins of spirituality in Human Evolution
Glasses For Enhanced Visual Acuity
Stimulating My Brain As A Spiritual Path
Sex and States of Consciousness
The Gay Male Brain - Evolutionary Speculations
Odd Experiences - Online Poll Results
Near-Death Experiences - Thai Case histories
Meditations from Brain Science